PortadaGruposCharlasMásPanorama actual
Buscar en el sitio
Este sitio utiliza cookies para ofrecer nuestros servicios, mejorar el rendimiento, análisis y (si no estás registrado) publicidad. Al usar LibraryThing reconoces que has leído y comprendido nuestros términos de servicio y política de privacidad. El uso del sitio y de los servicios está sujeto a estas políticas y términos.

Resultados de Google Books

Pulse en una miniatura para ir a Google Books.

The Political Mind: Why You Can't Understand…
Cargando...

The Political Mind: Why You Can't Understand 21st-Century American Politics with an 18th-Century Brain (edición 2008)

por George Lakoff (Autor)

MiembrosReseñasPopularidadValoración promediaMenciones
473852,150 (3.69)8
In What's the Matter with Kansas?, Thomas Frank pointed out that a great number of Americans actually vote against their own interests. In The Political Mind, George Lakoff explains why.
Miembro:davibear
Título:The Political Mind: Why You Can't Understand 21st-Century American Politics with an 18th-Century Brain
Autores:George Lakoff (Autor)
Información:Viking Adult (2008), 304 pages
Colecciones:Tu biblioteca
Valoración:
Etiquetas:SJT

Información de la obra

The Political Mind: Why You Can't Understand 21st-Century American Politics with an 18th-Century Brain por George Lakoff

Ninguno
Cargando...

Inscríbete en LibraryThing para averiguar si este libro te gustará.

Actualmente no hay Conversaciones sobre este libro.

» Ver también 8 menciones

Mostrando 1-5 de 8 (siguiente | mostrar todos)
I was drawn to this book after having read "Metaphors We Live By" and "Where Mathematics Comes From: How the Embodied Mind Brings Mathematics into Being", both of which Lakoff co-wrote.

This is a 2008 book about the rapidly changing field of neuroscience, I've now read it in 2024, so I can't be sure if its details are up to date. However, Lakoff presents interesting models of how the brain works and its relevance to politics in the United States. There are lots of helpful examples explaining how his concepts work in real life and in politics. He is careful to tell when something is still a theory that may need to be modified as more research is done.

I can't claim to have read and re-read each chapter in detail, but Lakoff seems to sometimes use more than one term for the same concept, and its not always clear to me what he means by important terms like 'event' or 'frame' or 'semantic role'. When I look in the index for help with many terms used in the book, I fail to find at least half of them.

Still, this is an important book, and I'm glad I read it. ( )
  mykl-s | Apr 14, 2024 |
Ever wonder why people are not logical and rational like you? Well they are - and you aren't as rational and logical as you think. Open your mind to the implications of keeping your authoritarian father frame instead of a nurturing parent frame.
  JeffcoHumanists | Mar 10, 2019 |
Humans are not rational animals, at least not in traditional Enlightenment view that they make choices based on reason, intellect, logic, facts, and evidence. Vulcans may do that, but humans don't, not even in Star Trek. Perhaps we'd be better off if we did, but that's an entirely different discussion, and not one that Lakoff argues in this book. He's addressing how humans actually think, not whether it's good or bad. It's real, and it's important to make that reality the foundation of how to frame political discussions. As this was written at the end of the GW Bush administration, the examples he uses are dated, and many of the points he makes in this book are the same as those in his later book [b:The ALL NEW Don't Think of an Elephant!: Know Your Values and Frame the Debate|23392815|The ALL NEW Don't Think of an Elephant! Know Your Values and Frame the Debate|George Lakoff|https://images.gr-assets.com/books/1423600757s/23392815.jpg|2187692]. If you've read that, you can probably give this one a miss. ( )
  DLMorrese | Aug 23, 2017 |
This is a pretty good read with some pretty good cognitive science research. It was nothing ground-breaking but the information was good. As someone who is a progressive, it's a bit sad to not really see it applied, 4 years after the book comes out. ( )
  g33kgrrl | Jun 30, 2013 |
I was drawn to this book mostly because I knew of the author’s reputation as a cognitive scientist and as someone who was known for spelling out how cognitive science overlaps with, and largely explains, many of the phenomena that we recognize as falling along the left-right spectrum of political ideologies. And Lakoff certainly does offer some insights into how thinking occurs, and what in particular is unique about the way we think about political issues.

Lakoff’s main idea, which should be apparent to anyone who watches endless hours of cable hours in rapture as I do, is that whoever controls the narrative frames of a debate controls the issue itself, and therefore always wins. How is this the case? Lakoff says that our view of rationality is largely, and erroneously, informed by the Enlightenment, which assumed it was conscious, universal, disembodied, logical, unemotional, value-neutral, interest-based, and literal. He shows several reasons why almost none of these are actually true. For example, we make decisions, to help others perhaps, that don’t actually maximize our own self-interest, and that are tied up with value and emotional content.

He claims that Democrats - very often with a grating, whiny tone - remain stuck in this view of rationality. Because of this, they are still in the habit of trying to lasso the facts, build charts and models, and explain why Republicans are simply wrong on many of the issues. Lakoff claims that this just isn’t enough. Evidence, reason, facts, and figures won’t win debates, he claims. But Republicans have learned how human reason really works – that it is in fact couched in tropes, metaphors, emotional phrases and associations – and they use them to their advantage in shaping political issues and talking points. Republicans just couch the issue in the terms that will helps them (their “narrative frames”), and then repeat that frame over and over again until it sticks in the minds of the public. Once stuck, it’s difficult, but not impossible, to dislodge. But doing so would just be a matter of finding the right frame that speaks to your political basis, and saying it repeatedly.

For example, conservatives have controlled the ideological frame regarding the “war on terror” for the last decade, and therefore they control many of the issues that we associate with “homeland security” (another phrase unused unquestioningly, according to Lakoff, that plays into Republican and neoconservative hands). Instead of accepting the frames of questions like “Do you think we should continue to fight the war on terror, or pull out?” or “Should middle class tax cuts be extended, or should they get a tax hike?” the issues need to be reformulated to emphasis what Lakoff thinks are the values of liberals and progressives: fairness, equality, and government accountability. In other words, re-frame the issues in such a way that benefits your own positions. And then repeat that framing. Over and over and over and over again – because, according to him, that’s the only way you’re going to win the debate.

There are a lot of problems with this book, though. With an objective-sounding title like “The Political Mind: A Cognitive Scientist’s Guide to Your Brain and Its Politics,” I didn’t really want any of Lakoff’s partisan comments. I knew before opening the book that he’s a committed liberal (many of my own sympathies, too, are very much left of center), but he spends too much time demonizing one political perspective, glorifying another, and too little time providing details and supporting evidence for the claims that he’s making. I feel that this saps the book of almost all of its credibility. In order to have a book be a powerful explanatory tool, instead of a passing as a fat pamphlet for the Obama campaign, it should stick to the facts of the matter accompanied, perhaps, with some reasonable inductions, predictions, and details of methodological practice. Calling President Bush a “traitor” (which he actually does) accomplishes nothing. Furthermore, it made me realize more and more as I read it that this book is simply an example of what he was talking about: a successful example of framing issues in an advantageous way. Of course, he would be the last person to actually bring that to the reader’s attention.

I think the book may have suffered from being written for too popular an audience, too. It seems that the political potshots were filler for an audience who was more eager to see their opponent trashed than to actually read something about how cognitive science can help us better understand how we think metaphorically about political issues. I came really close to giving this two stars, which I almost never do, but thought there were a couple of insights that salvaged it from being a total loss, so I opted for three stars instead. For someone interested in this topic, I suggest looking elsewhere. ( )
3 vota kant1066 | Aug 9, 2012 |
Mostrando 1-5 de 8 (siguiente | mostrar todos)
sin reseñas | añadir una reseña
Debes iniciar sesión para editar los datos de Conocimiento Común.
Para más ayuda, consulta la página de ayuda de Conocimiento Común.
Título canónico
Título original
Títulos alternativos
Fecha de publicación original
Personas/Personajes
Lugares importantes
Acontecimientos importantes
Películas relacionadas
Epígrafe
Dedicatoria
Primeras palabras
Citas
Últimas palabras
Aviso de desambiguación
Editores de la editorial
Blurbistas
Idioma original
DDC/MDS Canónico
LCC canónico

Referencias a esta obra en fuentes externas.

Wikipedia en inglés (1)

In What's the Matter with Kansas?, Thomas Frank pointed out that a great number of Americans actually vote against their own interests. In The Political Mind, George Lakoff explains why.

No se han encontrado descripciones de biblioteca.

Descripción del libro
Resumen Haiku

Debates activos

Ninguno

Cubiertas populares

Enlaces rápidos

Valoración

Promedio: (3.69)
0.5
1 1
1.5 1
2 4
2.5 1
3 12
3.5 7
4 23
4.5 1
5 11

¿Eres tú?

Conviértete en un Autor de LibraryThing.

 

Acerca de | Contactar | LibraryThing.com | Privacidad/Condiciones | Ayuda/Preguntas frecuentes | Blog | Tienda | APIs | TinyCat | Bibliotecas heredadas | Primeros reseñadores | Conocimiento común | 204,758,758 libros! | Barra superior: Siempre visible